Skip to main content

Subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract (Catha edulis forsk) induces schizophernic-like symptoms in mice

Abstract

Background

Chewing fresh leaves of the khat plant (Catha edulis forsk) is a deep rooted and widespread habit in East Africa and the Middle East. Although a body of knowledge exists about the adverse effects of khat on health, data are sparse with regard to the consequences of long-term khat chewing in resulting schizophrenic like symptoms.

Methods

A crude extract of khat at different doses (100 mg/kg (K (khat)100), 200 mg/kg (K200) and 400 mg/kg (K400)) were administered for experimental group of mice whereas standard (ketamine (KT) 10 mg/kg- positive controls (KT10)) and vehicle (2 % v/v Tween-80 in distilled water – negative control groups (CON)) were administered for control groups of mice daily for two months to evaluate subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract to induce schizophrenic-like symptoms in mice. Mice were subjected to a battery of behavioural tests and parameters like locomotor activity, total time spent in social interaction and level of cognition among different groups of mice were measured and analyzed.

Results

Khat at all doses significantly increased (p < 0.001) the mean locomotor activity score of mice compared to CON. However, the mean locomotor activity score of mice treated with khat was significantly lower (p < 0.001) compared to the mean locomotor activity score of KT10 mice (p < 0.001).

The mean total time score (in seconds) spent in social interaction, mean total time score (in seconds) spent in sniffing and following the partner was significantly higher (p < 0.001) in CON groups of mice compared to khat and ketamine treated groups. Moreover, in spatial memory task, the mean latency score (in seconds) to find the platform of khat and ketamine treated mice was significantly higher (p < 0.05) when compared to CON.

Conclusions

Subchronic oral administration of khat showed an enhanced locomotor activity, reduced social interaction and impaired cognitive function, which demonstrated that long-term use of khat is associated with schizophernic-like symptoms.

Peer Review reports

Background

According to World Health Organization (WHO) definition, schizophrenia is a severe mental disorder, characterized by profound disruptions in thinking, affecting language, perception, and the sense of self and it affects more than 21 million people worldwide [1]. Since no biological marker for schizophernia has yet been found, diagnosis is based on the assessment of the symptoms of each patient [2]. Different studies have shown that khat chewing is associated with occurrence of psychotic symptoms [3, 4].

Chewing the fresh leaves and shoots of the ever green plant khat (Catha edulis) dates back several centuries in eastern and southern Africa and the Arabian Peninsula [5]. Nowadays, it is estimated that 20 million people worldwide chew khat regularly to enjoy its psychostimulant effects [6].

Studies have shown that neurocognitive impairments [5], euphoria, excitability, anxiety, and insomnia [7, 8], are some of central nervous system effects of khat chewing. Khat chewing can also induce psychological dependency [5, 9, 10].

The main psycho-active component of khat leaves is cathinone (S-(−)alpha-aminopropiophenone) [11]. Cathinone resembles amphetamine in chemical structure and affects the central and peripheral nervous system [12, 13] and behavior [14, 15]. Similarly amphetamine and some of its derivatives have been shown to induce psychotic symptoms in experimental settings in humans [16, 17] and animals [18] and have been known to exacerbate psychotic states in psychiatric patients [19, 20].

To the best of our knowledge, the relationship between use of khat and psychiatric problems has been little explored in population-based studies. It has been postulated that khat use can exacerbate psychotic symptoms in individuals with pre-existing conditions, and precipitate psychiatric disorders in vulnerable subjects [14]. Moreover, khat consumption may affect the course of a psychotic disorder [16, 17].

Establishment of animal models of schizophrenia is critical for both understanding the mechanisms underlying severe mental disease and developing new antipsychotics [21]. There are various animal models of schizophrenia which seek to replicate the symptoms that are observed in human schizophrenics [21]. However, it has been difficult to find a particular model that is able to completely replicate all the symptoms observed in humans. In animal models, hyperactivity is considered as representative of positive symptoms in humans. Social withdrawal is considered to be the animal version of negative symptoms [21].

The current animal study was conducted in order to show interaction between khat use and schizophernia as there is no large-scale studies examining the relationship between effect of khat and schizophrenic-like symptoms. Therefore, the aim of this study was to evaluate schizophrenic-like symptoms of subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract in mice.

Methods

Drugs and chemicals

Diethyl ether and chloroform (Sigma-Aldrich, Germany), ketamine (Trittau, Germany) and 70 % ethanol (Pharmaceutical Fund Supply Agency (PFSA), Addis Ababa, Ethiopia) were purchased from the respective local vendors.

Animals

Adult Albino mice bred in the animal house of the School of Pharmacy, Addis Ababa University and having weights ranging from 25 to 35 g and 8 weeks of age were used for the experiment. The animals were housed in groups of six in plastic wire meshed cages in the animal house. Wooden material used as bedding was replaced every 3 days. The animals were allowed free access to tap water and standard laboratory pellet. The environmental conditions were kept normal (25 ± 1 °C, and 12 h/12 h light/dark cycle). The study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of School of Pharmacy, Addis Ababa University, Ethiopia and the care and handling of mice were done in accordance with the internationally accepted standard guidelines for use of animals [20].

Plant material

Bundles of Catha edulis forsk shoots and small branches (about 8000 g) were purchased fresh at a local market in “Aweday”, one of the common natural habitat, which is located about 500 km away from Addis Ababa, in the Eastern part of Ethiopia. The bundles of khat plant were collected in December 2011 where large amounts of khat plant come to the local market. The fresh bundles were packed in plastic bags and transported in an ice box to the laboratory. The fresh leaves were immediately kept in a deep freezer (−20 °C). The plant was identified by a taxonomist and a voucher specimen was deposited in the National Herbarium, College of Natural Sciences, Addis Ababa University under a voucher Number 001.

Preparation of plant extracts (crude extract)

The extract was prepared as described by Connor et al. [22]. The leaves were finely chopped in a dark place, weighed and placed in an Erlenmeyer flask containing reagent grade chloroform and diethyl ether in a 1: 3 v/v ratio. The volume of the combination of these volatile solvents used was more than that needed to just cover the minced leaves in the flask; hence a total of 400_ml solvents (100_ml of chloroform and 300_ml of diethyl ether) used for 200_g of the chopped leaves and shoots.

The mixture of khat plant with the solvent system was shaken using rotary shaker (120 rev/min) for two days under controlled temperature (20°c). The extract was decanted and filtered with Whatman No. 1 filter paper. The extract was placed in a hood under pressure for a week and then dried using Lyophilizer (Operan, Korea). Extracts were kept covered with aluminum foil and refrigerated.

Induction of schizophernia

A study has shown that the most representative animal model of schizophrenic dysfunction seems to be those involving the administration of N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) antagonist. Thus animals which were treated with NMDA antagonists e.g., ketamine could be used as a model of several aspects of schizophrenic dysfunction in humans [21]. Accordingly, Albino mice were injected intraperitoneally with 10 mg/kg ketamine for consecutive 14 days.

Experimental design

A total of 60 albino mice were utilized and the animals were randomly divided into 5 groups of 12 animals (6 mice housed per cage) in each group.

  • Group I- Normal mice (received the vehicle, 2 % v/v Tween-80 in distilled water, p.o [CON])

  • Group II- Schizophernic control (received ketamine 10_mg/kg, i.p [KT10])

  • Group III- Khat treated (received 100_mg/kg, p.o [K100])

  • Group IV- Khat treated (received 200_mg/kg, p.o [K200])

  • Group V- Khat treated (received 400_mg/kg, p.o [K400])

The extract was administered to the respective groups through oral route using intragastric tube (gavage) for 60 days. Such route was used since pharmacokinetics studies showed that C. edulis or S-(−)-cathinone is readily absorbed into plasma from the stomach and mouth [23, 24]. Besides, C. edulis leaves are usually taken orally in humans. Throughout the experimental period, all extract solutions were made fresh and containers including syringes were covered with aluminum foil to avoid light decomposition. On each day of the experiment, experimental mice were taken from their cage and weighed using electrical digital balance with 0.01 precision, since the weight of animals was necessary to determine the doses of khat extract.

Dose selection was made based up on a previous study done by Admassie et al. [25]. The dose of the extract required was determined based on the weight of the mice. Total weight of mice taking similar dose of extract was determined for ease of dose calculation. Once total amount of extract administered was calculated, the extract was reconstituted with 2 % v/v Tween-80 in distilled water. The volume of reconstitution fluid was adjusted, so that the maximum volume administered should not normally exceed 1 ml at a time for individual mouse in accordance with the Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) guidelines for the testing of chemicals No. 413 ‘routes of exposure and dose administration considerations’: (as revised in 2010). CON group received only the reconstitution fluid based on their weight. This procedure was repeated every day for 60 days of experiment.

Timeline of the study design

After mice were administered crude khat extract for 60 consecutive days, they were subjected to a battery of behavioural tests (locomotor activity study, social interaction study and cognitive function study). During all the behavioural studies, no extract treatment was given.

For each of the behavioral studies, 6 animals were utilized at a time out of 12 animals from their respective groups (CON, KT10 and khat treated). Hence, the same mice were utilized in different behavioural studies at different time.

Locomotor activity study

The Linton instrumentation Activity Monitor (AM1053) (Palgrave, North folk), computer software coupled device, is used to record the activity score of mice. The device operates using an array of infrared beams and detectors.

Two activity boxes were placed in a quiet room. The boxes were transparent polycarbonate cages, which were similar to the home cages but were each equipped with 48 infrared beams, 24 on each level arranged in an 8x16, 1” (25.4_mm) pitched grid. A computer detected the disruption of the infrared beams and recorded the number of beam breaks. This beam breaks were considered as measures of locomotor activity.

Mice were habituated for two days to the testing room and the testing boxes (30 min/day for 2 days). Following habituation on day 1, mouse was placed into the locomotor activity boxes for 20 min and locomotor activity (number of infrared beam breaks) was measured. After each group study, the activity boxes were cleaned by 70 % ethanol to avoid any potential cues and make the activity box ready for the other group study. This procedure was repeated for another 4 days (a total of 5 testing days).

Social interaction study

The apparatus used for the detection of changes in social behavior consists of an open-topped apparatus (51 × 51 cm and 20 cm high). Mouse used in this study was housed individually for 5 days. Mice were marked with different colours to distinguish the different groups and placed into the box (with 60 W bright illumination 17 cm above and video camera mounted above so as to view the entire apparatus).

One mouse from each group was taken at a time and social activity studies were done for each mouse individually over a 10- minute period by remote video recording. Once social activity study was done for a group, the open-topped box was cleaned with 70 % ethanol to avoid any potential cues and make the box ready for the other group study.

Total time spent in social interaction, total time spent sniffing and following a partner within an inch and half were also determined in the present study. Total time spent in social interaction was determined by timing the sniffing of partner, crawling under or climbing over the partner, genital investigation of partner and following the partner. Total time spent sniffing and following a partner were determined by timing the sniffing and following of the partner within an inch and half respectively.

Cognitive study

Morris water maze (MWM) was used to evaluate cognitive function [26]. MWM consisted of a circular pool (122 cm diameter and 76 cm depth) in which mice were trained to escape from water by swimming to a hidden platform (1.5 cm beneath water surface) whose location could be only identified using distal extra-maze cues attached to the room walls. Different 3-D shapes that can act as visual cues were kept at constant location during the whole experiment. Water temperature was maintained at 21 ± 1 °C. The platform was placed in the middle of the North-West (NW) quadrant and remained at the same position during the whole experiment.

Acquisition phase study

The spatial acquisition phase consisted of 16 training trials: 4 training trials per day and 4 training days with an inter-trial interval of 20 min. Mice were released randomly with their heads facing the pool wall from the four compass locations (NE, NW, SW, and SE), and allowed to swim and search for the platform for 120 s. If mice did not locate the platform after 120 s, animals were manually placed on the platform and allowed to remain on it for 30 s so that learning had to occur.

On the first training day, mice were given an acclimatization training session in the water maze by placing on the hidden platform and after acclimatization, mice were allowed to swim for 30 s and the latency to reach the hidden platform was recorded. And, if the mice were unable to found the hidden platform, they were guided back to the platform. This study was done for another 3 days (a total of 4 days) and the latency to reach the hidden platform in each day was recorded.

Short-term memory retention phase study

One day after the acquisition phase (i.e., on day 5), subjects received a probe trial, in which the platform was removed. Mice were released from the NW start point and were allowed to swim freely for 60 s. Time spent in target quadrant (where the platform was located) was recorded.

Long-term memory retention phase study

On the 12th day, subjects once again received the probe trial for 60 s to check retention of memory. Mice were released from the NW start point and were allowed to swim freely for 60 s. Time spent in target quadrant was recorded. Mice didn’t receive training between 5 and 12 days.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS software package, Version 20.0. All the data were expressed as mean ± SEM. In order to study locomotor activity and social interaction among groups of mice, one way with Tukey’s procedure and repeated measures of ANOVA were utilized while for the study of cognition, one way repeated measure of ANOVA and paired sampled t-test was used. The value of p < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Locomotor activity among groups of mice

A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to compare the locomotor activity scores of control, khat treated and ketamine groups of mice after subchronic crude khat extract administration daily for 2 months. ANOVA test indicated that there was a significant difference in the mean locomotor activity scores (across the five test days) of control groups (CON and KT10) and khat treated groups of mice (K100, K200 and K400) (F (4, 25) =136.17, p < 0.001) (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean locomotor activity score of groups of mice during the 20 min trial in the Activity Device Monitor across the five test days (n = 6/group). Data analysis were done by using Tukey’s procedure. **p < 0.001, indicates level of significance relative to controls (CON)

A one-way repeated measures ANOVA was also conducted to compare the mean locomotor activity score of khat treated and ketamine groups of mice during the 20 min trial in the device across the five test days. There was a significant effect for days, Wilks’ Lambda = 0.58, F (4, 22) = 3.94, p = 0.015, multivariate partial squared = 0.42. This result suggest that the mean locomotor activity score of khat treated and ketamine groups of mice had significant effect for days.

Repeated measure of ANOVA also revealed that there was a significant effect between groups (CON and khat treated) of mice, F (4, 25) =135.82, p < 0.001. However, within-groups (days* group) effect of mean locomotor activity score was not significant, F (16, 100) = 1.25, p =0.246. This means that the mean locomotor activity score of each mice within respective groups of mice were not significantly different across the five test days.

Social interaction

Total time spent in social interaction

A one-way analysis of variance was conducted to compare the total time spent in social interaction scores of control, khat and ketamine treated groups of mice after subchronic crude khat extract administration daily for 2 months.

ANOVA test indicated that there was a significant difference in the mean total time spent in social interaction scores of control groups (CON and KT10) and khat treated groups of mice (K100, K200 and K400) (F (4, 25) =244.16, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean total time score (in seconds) spent in social interaction among groups of mice over the 10 min video recording in open-topped box (n = 6/group). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the khat extract dose-dependently decreased the mean total time spent in social interaction (p < 0.001). *p < 0.001, indicates level of significance relative to controls (CON)

Total time spent sniffing the partner

A one-way analysis of variance was conducted to compare the total time spent in sniffing scores of control, khat treated and ketamine groups of mice after subchronic crude khat extract administration daily for 2 months. ANOVA test indicated that there was a significant difference in the mean total time score spent in sniffing partner within an inch and half of control groups (CON and KT10) and khat treated groups of mice (K100, K200 and K400) (F (4, 25) = 126.23, p < 0.001) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean total time score (in seconds) spent in sniffing partner within an inch and half of groups of mice over the 10 min video recording in open-topped box (n = 6/group). *p < 0.001; indicates level of significance relative to controls (CON)

Pairwise comparisons of mean total time score (seconds) spent sniffing partner showed that when the different doses of crude extracts were compared to each other, the highest dose, K400, resulted in the lowest total mean time score spent in sniffing the partner compared to K200 (p = 0.014) and K100 doses (p < 0.001). However, pairwise comparisons among K200 and K100 doses were not significant (p = 0.57).

Total time spent following a partner

A one-way analysis of variance was also conducted to compare the total time score spent in following a partner of control, khat treated and ketamine groups of mice after subchronic crude khat extract administration daily for 2 months. ANOVA test indicated that there was a significant difference in the mean total time score spent in following partner of control groups (CON and KT10) and khat treated groups of mice (K100, K200 and K400) (F (4, 25) = 115.60, p < 0.001) (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean total time score (in seconds) spent in following partner within an inch and half of groups of mice over the 10 min video recording in open-topped box (n = 6/ group). *p < 0.001, indicates level of significance relative to controls (CON)

Pairwise comparisons of mean total time score (seconds) spent following partner demonstrated that comparisons among different doses of the extract on the mean total time score spent following the partner revealed that the decrease was dose-dependent. Accordingly, K400 resulted in the lowest total mean time score spent in following the partner compared to K200 (p = 0.002) and K100 doses (p < 0.001). However, pairwise comparisons among K200 and K100 doses were not significant (p = 0.10).

Cognition

The present results demonstrated the performance of khat treated and control groups of mice in water based spatial task. All groups of mice learned the task in the MWM within 16 trials. During the training most of khat as well as ketamine treated groups of mice preferred the edge of the wall and circled swimming in the direction submerged. In identification of the hidden platform, khat treated and ketamine groups of mice were able to touch the platform but unable to climb.

Acquisition phase

The performance of acquisition phase was confirmed by applying the general linear model: repeated measures of ANOVA between the latency to reach the hidden platform and days of trials. The mean latency score of each group of mice during each test days are presented in Fig. 5. There was a significant effect for days, Wilks’ Lambda = 0.56, F (3, 23) = 6.14, p = 0.003, multivariate partial squared = 0.45. This result suggest that the mean latency score of khat treated and ketamine groups of mice had significant effect for days.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean latency score to reach the hidden platform (acquisition phase) among groups of mice over the period of four days of water maze trials (n = 6/ group)

Repeated measure of ANOVA also revealed that there was a significant effect between groups (CON and khat treated) of mice, F (4, 25) =351.20, p < 0.001, multivariate partial squared = 0.98. Likewise, within-groups (days* group) effect of mean latency score to reach the hidden platform was significant, F (12, 75) =9.71, p < 0.001, multivariate partial squared = 0.61. This means that the mean latency to reach the hidden platform of each mice within respective groups of mice were significantly different across the five test days.

The mean difference between the latency score to reach the hidden platform on day1 and day 4 (∆L) was calculated for each khat treated and controls groups of mice in order to study their ability to learn the task. The means and SEM are presented in Table 1. One way ANOVA analysis indicated that there was a significant difference in the mean latency score to reach the hidden platform of CON and other groups of mice (K100, K200, K400 and KT10) (F (4, 25) = 26.73, p < 0.001). This indicated that CON group learns the task but other groups of mice failed to learn the task.

Table 1 The difference between mean latency score to reach the hidden platform on day 1 and day 4

Short-term memory retention phase

During the probe trial on day 5, time (in seconds) spent in the target and in the adjacent quadrants was analyzed using paired sample t-test and repeated measure of ANOVA. The result of the repeated measures of ANOVA indicated that there was a significant effect for quadrant (either target or adjacent quadrant) preferences, (Wilks’ Lambda = 0.03, F (4, 25) = 960.20, p < 0.001, multivariate partial squared = 0.98).

The paired sample t-test (for each group separately) was also performed to confirm that the preference between the mean time score spent in the target and adjacent quadrants among different groups of mice. Accordingly, CON group of mice spent significantly more time in target quadrant (p < 0.001, t = 16.94) (Mean = 37.12, SEM = 1.17, N = 6) compared to adjacent quadrant (mean = 17.40, SEM = 0.00, N = 6). The increase was 19.77, with 95 % confidence interval between the means of 16.77 to 22.77 while K100 (t = 3.67, p = 0.014), K200 (t = 18.04, p < 0.001), K400 (t = 18.69, p < 0.001) and KT10 (t = 46.71, p < 0.001) groups of mice significantly spent more time in adjacent quadrant (See Fig. 6).

Fig. 6
figure 6

The mean time score spent in target and adjacent quadrants among different groups of mice during the probe trial on Day 5 (n = 6/ group). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001; indicates level of significance for each group separately

Long-term memory retention phase

During the probe trial on day 12, time (in seconds) spent in the target and in the adjacent quadrants was analyzed using paired sample t-test and repeated measure of ANOVA. The result of the repeated measures of ANOVA indicated that there was a significant effect for quadrant (target and adjacent quadrants) preferences, (Wilks’ Lambda = 0.06, F (4, 25) = 366.04, p < 0.001, multivariate partial squared = 0.94).

The paired sample t-test (for each group separately) was also performed to confirm that the preference between the mean time score spent in the target and adjacent quadrants of different groups of mice. Accordingly, CON groups of mice spent significantly more time in target quadrant (p < 0.001, t = 6.73) (Mean = 34.33, SEM = 1.58, N = 6) compared to adjacent quadrant (mean = 23.67, SEM = 0.00, N = 6). The increase was 10.66, with 95 % confidence interval between the means of 6.59 to 14.74 while K100 (t = 6.95, p = 0.001), K200 (t = 20.43, p < 0.001), K400 (t = 38.33, p < 0.001) and KT10 (t = 66.48, p < 0.001) groups of mice significantly spent more time in adjacent quadrant (See Fig. 7).

Fig. 7
figure 7

The mean time score spent in target and adjacent quadrants among different groups of mice during the probe trial on Day 12 (n = 6/ group). *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001; indicates level of significance for each group separately

Discussion

This study described the schizophrenic-like symptoms of subchronic (two months) oral administration of crude khat extract (Catha edulis forsk) in mice using battery of behavioural tests (locomotor activity study, social interaction study and cognitive function study). The study revealed that subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract daily resulted in dose-related neurobehavioural abnormalities, including locomotor hyperactivity, abnormal social behavior and decreased cognition. These abnormal behaviors in mice are thought to be correlates of the symptoms of schizophrenia [27].

In this study, the mean locomotor activity score of mice treated with khat was significantly higher compared to control (p < 0.001) whereas it was significantly lower compared to ketamine treated groups of mice (p < 0.001). The tendency of increased locomotor activity with khat administration is concordant with previous studies [28, 29]. This phenomenon is assumed to be due to neurotransmitter systems, including dopamine and serotonin, as locomotor activities are modulated by these neurotransmitters. Cathinone, the active compound of khat is associated directly and/or indirectly with dopamine or serotonin release by its action on dopamine or serotonin transporter function [29, 30]. Further, it has been demonstrated that the motor activities induced by S – (−) cathinone in experimental animals are associated with dopamine release [29].

Habituation refers to a decrease in response that is observed when an identical stimulus is presented repeatedly and it is considered to be the simplest form of learning [31]. Interestingly, a deficit in this form of non-associative learning has been demonstrated in this study and the mean locomotory effect of control mice across the five test days decreased while this was not true for khat and ketamine treated mice. This might be probably because of control mice developed habituation of the device within test days. However, for those groups treated with khat and ketamine, the observed effect might be due to either neurochemical effect outweighs habituation of the activity device monitor or there was a deficit in non-associative learning.

Results from current study showed that subchronic oral administration of crude C. edulis extract induced the locomotor-activity in a dose-dependent manner. This finding is concordant with earlier study that demonstrated animals behave in the same way when administered cathinone as if they had been given (+)-amphetamine, and the response was dose related [32]. Animal study has also shown that repeated administration of amphetamine-like stimulants result in altered behavioural responses like behavioural sensitization [33]. Behavioural sensitization occurs when repeated treatment with psychostimulant drugs produces alteration in the behaviour that outlasts the initial neuropharmacological actions [33]. In this regard, khat extract in current study might exhibit behavioural sensitization on locomotion, ultimately leading to locomotor sensitization.

Psychosis and behavioural sensitization in laboratory animals are frequently correlated [34]. Therefore, in this study, the phenomenon of locomotor sensitization induced by C. edulis extract or ketamine in mice may bear striking similarities to the progressive development of psychosis in human. For example, different clinical studies revealed the association between repeated C. edulis chewing and psychosis [14, 15, 35]. Hence, the use of psychostimulants is often associated with a higher risk of psychosis [36, 37]. Taken together, the capacity of C. edulis to elicit a long-lasting behavioural sensitization supports the anecdotal reports about psychiatric problems which was associated with C. edulis chewing [15, 38]. Recently, it is also argued that organophosphates (through the exposure of khat to pesticides during or after cultivation) and high level of free radicals that could be linked directly to the effects of the khat constituents, cathinone and cathine, are important confounders in khat chronic consumption and psychosis symptoms in human [39].

It’s known that social withdrawal is considered to be the animal version of negative symptoms of schizophrenia [21]. The present study revealed the mean total time score spent in social interaction of khat and ketamine treated mice was significantly lower than control groups. Thus, khat treated groups were socially isolated than controls taking the vehicle but ketamine treated mice were highly isolated socially. This result is in line with the fact that schizophrenics experience symptoms subtracted from normal behaviour or function, for example social withdrawal, blunted affect and lack of motivation [21].

The current study also showed that after subchronic oral administration of crude C. edulis extract, the mean time (sec) spent in social interaction decreased in a dose ̱ dependent manner. This may be possibly explained because of khat-induced over stimulation of D2 receptors resulted in hypostimulation of D1 receptors which has been suggested to be in part responsible for negative symptoms of schizophrenia [40]. Furthermore, since the seminal work of Pycock et al.[41], many laboratories result have described reciprocal and opposite regulations between D2 and D1 systems. Hence, higher khat-induced over stimulation of D2 receptors because of the higher dose of crude khat extract administered (400 mg/kg) in current study may resulted in greater hypostimulation of D1 receptors.

According to Kwon et al., unfamiliar mice placed in a neutral arena will usually display high levels of sniffing and following [42]. In contrary, the results from this study indicated that time spent sniffing and following the partner within an inch and half showed decrement in a dose- dependent manner. This may possibly due to lack of motivation of mice taking the extract to investigate unfamiliar mice in the open box chamber indicating subtracted symptoms from normal behavior.

Since its first application in 1981, the MWM has become one of the most frequently used tool for analyzing spatial learning and memory [26]. The tool is known for its basic training protocols that include hidden-platform acquisition training, probe trial testing and working memory testing [26].

Our results indicate that there was mean improvement in overall performance of control mice in spatial learning whereas khat and ketamine treated mice showed very poor learning during the period of training. During the hidden-platform acquisition training, control mice learned the task with decreased latencies across time indicating mean improvement in the animal’s performance. The difference in latency to reach the platform between Day 1 and Day 4 was significantly higher in the controls indicating ability to learn the task. However, no significant learning was observed for either khat or ketamine treated mice during acquisition phase because of they showed no significant latency differences in the trials. This finding is consistent with a study that examined impaired cognitive function such as deficits in performance in spatial alternation tasks using ketamine in laboratory animals as compared to controls [43].

Previous preliminary observation of khat use suggests that chronic khat use is implicated with various cognitive and mental health impairments [18, 38]. However, extensive study on the effect of khat use on learning and memory is lacking. Therefore, the effect of khat use on learning and memory is usually deducted from studies made on other substances with close structural and functional similarity with the active constituent of khat. Amphetamine is the substance with close similarity to the active constituent of khat, cathinone [28]. A review by Warfa et al., indicated that chronic use of amphetamine leads to impairment on learning and memory, for instance, negative effect on episodic memory, working memory and a delay in the organizational and technical component of memory encoding [18].

In this study after a series of acquisition trial blocks, a probe or transfer trial at day 5 is usually performed to determine the spatial accuracy of the animal which is represented by the time it spends looking for the platform in the quadrant where the platform used to be (target quadrant) [44]. Accordingly, in the current study control group spent more time in target quadrant while K100, K200, K400 and KT10 groups spent more time in adjacent quadrants indicating that khat extract resulted in impairment of spatial accuracy to determine where the platform was used to be. The current study also indicated impairment in long term memory after probe trial at day 12 in MWM task. These all might be possibly resulted from the nature of cathinone. In CNS cathinone provokes the release of catecholamines especially dopamine at synaptic storage sites and results in overall stimulation of D2 receptors. This stimulation inhibits NMDA-mediated glutamate transmission and long term potentiation [45]. Also it is clear that glutamate and dopaminergic system are known to modulate each other’s activity in reciprocal fashion [46].

Conclusions

Looking at the data produced in the present study, subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract resulted in enhanced motor activity in ADM study, symptoms subtracted from normal behavior of mice in open box chamber study and cognitive deficit in MWM study. Based on these findings, subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract (Catha edulis forsk) induces schizophernic-like symptoms in mice.

Abbreviations

ADM, Activity device monitor; CNS, Central nervous system; MWM, Morris water maze; NMDA, N-methyl-D-aspartic acid; WHO, World Health Organization.

References

  1. Schizophrenia. [http://www.who.int/mental_health/management/schizophrenia/en/].

  2. Szymanski S, Kane JM, Lieberman JA. A selective review of biological markers in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1991;17(1):99–111.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Widmann M, Warsame AH, Mikulica J, et al. Khat use, PTSD and psychotic symptoms among Somali refugees in Nairobi – a pilot study. Front Public Health. 2014;2:71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Odenwald M, Neuner F, Schauer M, et al. Khat use as risk factor for psychotic disorders: a cross-sectional and case–control study in Somalia. BMC Med. 2005;3:5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Odenwald M, Klein A, Warfa N. In: Miller PM, editor. Khat addiction. Principles of Addiction: Comprehensive Addictive Behaviors and Disorders. USA: Elsevier; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  6. El-Menyar A, Mekkodathil A, Al-Thani H, Al-Motarreb A. Khat use: history and heart failure. Oman Med J. 2015;30(2):77–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Al-Motarreb A, Al-Habori M, Broadley KJ. Khat chewing, cardiovascular diseases and other internal medical problems: The current situation and directions for future research. J Ethnopharmacol. 2010;132(3):540–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cox G, Rampes H. Adverse effects of khat: a review. Adv Psychiatr Treat. 2003;9(6):456–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kassim S, Islam S, Croucher R. Validity and reliability of a Severity of Dependence Scale for khat (SDS-khat). J Ethnopharmacol. 2010;132(3):570–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Griffiths P, Gossop M, Wickenden S, Dunworth J, Harris K, Lloyd C. A transcultural pattern of drug use: qat (khat) in the UK. Br J Psychiatry. 1997;170:281–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Al-Hebshi NN, Skaug N. Khat (catha edulis)-an updated review. Addict Biol. 2005;10(4):299–307.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Al-Motarreb A, Baker K, Broadley KJ. Khat: Pharmacological and medical aspects and its social use in Yemen. Phytother Res. 2002;16(5):403–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Giannini A, Castellani S. A manic-like psychosis due to khat. J Toxicol Clin Toxicol. 1982;19(5):455–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Yousef G, Huq Z, Lambert T. Khat chewing as a cause of psychosis. Bri J Hosp Med (Lond). 1995;54(7):322–6.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Alem A, Shibre T. Khat induced psychosis and its medico-legal implication: a case report. Ethiop Med J. 1997;35:137–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Brenneisen R, Geisshusler S, Schorno X. Metabolism of cathinone to (−) - norephedrine and (−)-norpseudoephedrine. J Pharm Pharmacol. 1986;38(4):298–300.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Schechter MD. Dopaminergic nature of acute cathine tolerance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;36(4):817–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Warfa N, Klein A, Bhui K, Leavey G, Craig T, Stansfield SA. Khat use and mental illness. A critical review. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(2):309–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Powell KJ, Hori SE, Leslie R. Cognitive impairments in the STOP null mouse model of schizophrenia. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121(5):826–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Vogel HG. Drug discovery and evaluation: pharmacological assays. 2nd ed. Verlag Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer; 2008.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  21. Dawe GS, Hwang EHJ, Tan CH. Pathophysiology and animal models of schizophrenia. Ann Acad Med Singapore. 2009;38:425–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Connor JD, Rostom A, Makonnen E. Comparison of the effects of khat extracts and amphetamine on motor behaviors in mice. J Ethnopharmacol. 2002;81:65–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Widler P, Mathys K, Brenneisen R, Kalix P, Fisch HU. Pharmacodynamics and pharmacokinetics of khat: a controlled study. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 1994;55(5):556–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Toennes SW, Harder S, Schramm M, Niess C, Kauert GF. Pharmacokinetics of cathinone, cathine and norephedrine after the chewing of khat leaves. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2003;56:125–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Admassie E, Engidawork E. Subchronic administration of Catha edulis F. (khat) extract is marked by elevation of cardiac biomarkers and subendocardial necrosis besides blood pressure alteration in rats. J Ethnopharmacol. 2011;136:246–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Morris RG. Learning and motivation. J Biosci Tech. 1981;12:239–60.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Powell CM, Miyakawa T. Schizophrenia-relevant behavioral testing inrodent mode: ls a uniquely human disorder? Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:1198–207.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Wabe NT. Chemistry, pharmacology, and toxicology of khat (Catha Edulis Forsk): a review. Addict Health. 2011;3(3–4):137–49.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Lopez-Arnau R, Martinez-Clemente J, Pubill D, Escubedo E, Camarasa J. Comparative neuropharmacology of three psychostimulant cathinone derivatives: butylone, mephedrone and methylone. Br J Pharmacol. 2012;167(2):407–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Banjaw MY, Mayerhofer A, Schmidt WJ. Anticataleptic activity of cathinone and MDMA (Ecstasy) upon acute and subchronic administration in rat. Synapse. 2003;49:232–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Petrinovich L, Peeke HV. Habituation to territorial song in the White-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys). Behav Biol. 1973;8:743–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Izawa J, Yamanashi K, Asakura T, Misu Y, Goshima Y. Differential effects of methamphetamine and cocaine on behavior and extracellular levels of dopamine and 3,4-dihydroxyphenylalanine in the nucleus accumbens of conscious rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 2006;549(1–3):84–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Banjaw MY, Schmidt WJ. Behavioral sensitization following repeated intermittent oral administration of Catha edulis in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2005;156:181–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Robinson TE, Becker JB. Enduring changes in brain and behaviour produced by chronic amphetamine administration : a review and evaluation of animal models of amphetamine psychosis. Brain Res. 1986;11:157–98.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Pantelis C, Hindler CG, Taylor JC. Use and abuse of khat (Catha edulis): a review of the distribution, pharmacology, side effects and a description of psychosis attributed to khat chewing. Psychol Med. 1989;19(3):657–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Chen CK, Lin SK, Sham PC, Ball D, Loh EW, Hsiao CC, Chiang YL, Ree SC, Lee CH, Murray RM. Pre-morbid characteristics and co-morbidity of methamphetamine users with and without psychosis. Psychol Med. 2003;33:1407–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Srisurapanont M, Ali R, Marsden J, Sunga A, Wada K, Monteiro M. Psychotic symptoms in methamphetamine psychotic in-patients. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2003;6:347–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Awas M, Kebede D, Alem A. Major mental disorders in Butajira, Southern Ethiopia. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1999;397:56–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Aleryani SL, Aleryani RA, Al-Akwa AA. Khat a drug of abuse: roles of free radicals and antioxidants. Drug Test Anal. 2011;3(9):548–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Abi-Dargham A, Moore H. Prefrontal DA transmission at D1 receptors and the pathology of schizophrenia. Neuroscientist. 2003;9:404–16.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Pycock CJ, Kerwin RW, Carter CJ. Effect of lesion of cortical dopamine terminals on subcortical dopamine receptors in rats. Nature. 1980;286:74–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kwon CH, Luikart BW, Powell CM, Zhou J, Matheny SA, Zhang W, et al. Pten regulates neuronal arborization and social interaction in mice. Neuron. 2006;50(3):377–88.

  43. Moghaddam B, Jackson ME. Glutamatergic animal models of schizophrenia. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003:131–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Vorhees CV, Williams MT. Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory. Nat Protoc. 2006;1(2):848–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Centonze D, Picconi B, Gubellini P, Bernardi G, Calabresi P. Dopaminergic control of synaptic plasticity in the dorsal striatum. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13:1071–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Egan MF, Weinberger DR. Neurobiology of schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7:701–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are indebted to Addis Ababa University and Samara University, Ethiopia for providing financial support. And, we also thankful to Department of Pharmacology and Clinical Pharmacy, Addis Ababa University, Addis Ababa, Ethiopia for arranging the necessary materials and granting timely support at laboratory work. Our thanks also goes to Prof. Alemayehu Worku, Professor of Biostatistics and Public Health, School of Public Health, College of Health Sciences, Addis Ababa University, Addis Ababa, Ethiopia for his specialist and technical advice. Lastly, we are grateful to Fantu Assefa and Molla Wale for their technical assistance during the laboratory work.

Availability of data and materials

This information is not relevant.

Authors’ contributions

TB and EE contributed to the design of the study and the interpretation of data. TB and EY performed the data analysis and drafted the manuscript. All authors critically revised the draft manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

This information is not relevant.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of School of Pharmacy, Addis Ababa University, Ethiopia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tegegne Bogale.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bogale, T., Engidawork, E. & Yisma, E. Subchronic oral administration of crude khat extract (Catha edulis forsk) induces schizophernic-like symptoms in mice. BMC Complement Altern Med 16, 153 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12906-016-1145-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12906-016-1145-6

Keywords